About the presenter: Anthony J. Caruso, Ph.D., professor, School of Speech Pathology & Audiology , Kent State University, has been actively involved in the clinical management of adults and children who stutter for over 17 years.. He has published several journal articles and book chapters on the physiological events associated with fluent and stuttered speech. Caruso (with E. Strand) co-edited a recent book entitled "Clinical Management of Motor Speech Disorders in Children" (Thieme).

You can post Questions/comments about the following paper to Anthony Caruso before October 22, 2000.


Perspectives Regarding the Clinical Utility of a Speech Motor Perspective on Childhood Stuttering

by Anthony J. Caruso
from Ohio, USA

In a recent chapter, Caruso, Max & McClowry (1999) examine the term "motor speech disorders" with particular emphasis on childhood stuttering. Speech-language clinicians and researchers often distinguish among different communication disorders on the basis of the processes underlying those disorders. For example, the group of acquired neurogenic disorders known as the aphasias typically is described in terms of underlying deficits in various linguistic processes (e.g., agrammatism). Dysarthria and apraxia of speech, on the other hand, typically are described in terms of underlying disturbances in peripheral or central aspects of speech motor control. In light of this perspective, dysarthria and apraxia of speech are generally categorized as motor speech disorders (e.g., Duffy, 1995). In fact, these two disorders, in both their developmental and acquired forms, have become so tightly linked to the concept of speech motor disorders that they are often listed as the only examples for this class of speech disorders (e.g., Crary, 1995; Kent, 1990; Strand, 1995). Nevertheless, any speech disorder that is associated with either a nervous system lesion or a dysfunction of the sensorimotor processes underlying speech production could be correctly classified as a motor speech disorder. In this paper, we propose that currently available empirical data suggest that stuttering may be one example of a disorder that is based on a dysfunction, rather than lesion, of the central nervous system’s sensorimotor control mechanisms. Viewing stuttering as a motor speech disorder has important theoretical and clinical implications. Theoretically, it provides a framework that can be used to develop testable hypotheses regarding the nature of stuttering in children and adults. Clinically, this viewpoint has several benefits. First, it focuses treatment on the temporal and spatial requisites obligatory for fluent speech production. Second, it empowers clinicians to develop treatment protocols based on their understanding and working knowledge of sensorimotor contributions to speech motor output. Third, treatment planning from this perspective would increase the likelihood that alterations in the course of treatment for a particular client are consistent with what is known about programming and execution of fluent speech.

While relatively few studies have examined processes of speech motor control in children who stutter, there is some evidence that children who stutter exhibit difficulty in the planning or programming of speech movements. In light of the limitation on the length of this paper, we refer the reader to Caruso et al., 1999 for a review of motor speech studies in children who stutter. In light of these findings, it seems reasonable to incorporate methods or strategies to improve multiarticulate serial movement with children who stutter. In the remaining portion of this paper, the utility of applying what is known about human neuromotor movements and factors that influence motor performance will be discussed relative to childhood stuttering.

Clinical Utility of a Motor Speech Perspective on Childhood Stuttering

Viewing childhood stuttering as a motor speech disorder has several benefits for clinicians. Indeed, one major assumption underlying this approach is that young children who stutter unless changes are made in articulatory dynamics as well as the coordination of those dynamics with respiration and phonation will not achieve fluency. Second, it facilitates development of treatment protocols based upon principles of motor learning (Schmidt, 1982). Thus, changes in, for example, oral motor events for speech which are necessary for fluency, can be supported by treatment planning that parallels or is consistent with established theories in neuroscience, motor learning and speech motor control. One question we are often asked by clinicians is "Where and how do I begin getting this child to be fluent?" Clearly, this theoretically based treatment approach provides a framework that can benefit clinicians in their development of a course of treatment for a particular client. Third, fluency treatment that is based upon motor learning theory is advantageous to clinicians--particularly those clinicians who determine that, due to a child's lack of progress, alterations in treatment are warranted. Based on our observations of both professional and student clinicians, often times the difficulty for these clinicians is not a failure to recognize that changes in the course of treatment are warranted. Rather, our observations suggest that the difficulty for many fluency clinicians is how to alter treatment and remain consistent with the goals of therapy and the accepted finding regarding neuromotor processes under fluent speech.

The purpose of this paper is to encourage clinicians to adopt a motor speech perspective on stuttering therapy. We will discuss some general strategies toward childhood fluency therapy that are consistent with the perspectives previously discussed in this paper. Note, however, a thorough discussion of fluency treatment, and techniques to facilitate fluency are beyond the scope of this document. There are several sources available re: stuttering treatment which provide both broad and detailed information in this area (cf., Conture, 1990; Max & Caruso, 1997; Yaruss, 1997). Moreover, several authors have discussed the benefits of applying motor learning theory to clinical management of children with motor speech disorders in a recently published book (see Caruso & Strand, 1999). Specific to fluency disorders, we have discussed our views on motor learning theory in previous publications (Caruso & Max, 1997 ; Max, Caruso & Vandevenne, 1997). Our focus here is to provide information on aspects of fluency therapy that, in our view, are consistent with a motor speech/learning perspective on stuttering in order to encourage others to adopt and implement this perspective in treatment.

Rate of speech: "Slow" is better when learning a new skill.

As individuals learn novel tasks, or refine previously learned tasks, rate of performance has often been observed to be relatively slow in the beginning and speed up as skill level increases (Schmidt, 1988 ). One of the most robust findings in the stuttering treatment is that reduction of the client's speech production is an important variable in facilitating fluency. Although there are differing opinions as to why reduced rate of speech typically has a positive influence on fluency (cf. Ingham, 1990; Kalinowski, Armson & Stuart, 1995), there is general agreement that a slow rate permits more time for the speaker to program and/or execute speech movements and thus facilitates coordination for serial multiarticulate movements, respiration and coordination (e.g., Kent, 1984). It is interesting to note that stuttering, for most children, begins between 3 - 4 years of age, the same time of development that timing for speech typically becomes more refined (Netsell, 1981). It seems reasonable to suggest that children who stutter--i.e., children who are impaired in their abilities for speech timing and whose sensorimotor mechanism is still developing/evolving, would particularly benefit from a reduced rate of speech. The challenge to clinicians is that "slow" and "fast" are abstract concepts that may not be fully understood in young children near the onset of stuttering. Thus, some clinicians (see Conture, 1990) advocate pairing the abstract notion of "time" with concrete examples of "slow" and "fast"' and describe speech to the child as "turtle speech" or "rabbit speech."

It may be just as important to model a slow rate of speech for children who stutter as it is to have them reduce there own rate of speech. There is some evidence that disfluencies decrease when the communication partner's speech is slow (Guitar, Schaefer, Donahue-Kilburg & Bond , 1992). Moreover, this increase in fluency has occurred in conjunction with and increase in both the number of words per conversational turn and the number of conversational turns during the dyad (Caruso & Rolwing, in progress). Thus, it seems that children become more fluent as well as more verbal when speaking with a partner with a reduced speaking rate. One hypothesis regarding these results states that the partners model may provide a temporal model that the child who stutters utilizes in programming or planning speech movements (Caruso & Rolwing, in progress). For further information regarding different methods of rate reduction and the influence of the communication partner, see Logan & Caruso, 1997 and Max & Caruso, 1997).

Number of Movements

It is generally acknowledged in the nonspeech literature, that practicing a single novel movement facilitates motor learning for complex (e.g., multiple movements) tasks (Schmidt, 1988). Relative to speech, individuals who stutter appear to have an easier time producing, e.g., single word utterances versus multi-word phrases and sentences. Many therapies advocate starting at a single word or short phrase level and gradually increasing length and complexity of the utterance. Although it has its roots in operant conditioning, the fluency treatment by Ryan is consistent with this factor in motor learning (Ryan, 1974). Another way to manipulate the number of speech movements by the child who stutters involves the use of different question types by the communication partner. Use of different questions (asked by a communication partner) is one way to manipulate the number or words spoken by a child. Thus, to facilitate motor learning, a communication partner to provide an opportunity for the child to practice speech movements during fluency could utilize "low level demand" question (e.g., questions that can be answered with a single word response). Clinicians should assess this relationship between length of response (i.e., number of movements) and disfluency (Conture & Caruso, 1987) to determine when fluent ("typical") movements breakdown as the demand for an increase in number of speech movements increases by asking more open-ended questions. Communication partners can be trained to modify their question types to facilitate fluency when talking with children who stutter (Logan & Caruso,1997).

Other Variables Affecting Motor Learning: Implications for Fluency

Motor learning in general is known to be influenced by several factors: (1) the amount of repetitive practice; (2) feedback in the form of knowledge of results vs. knowledge of performance; (3) the schedule under which practice trials are performed, and, (4) motivation of the subject (see Caruso & Strand, 1999). Rarely have these variables been studied in stuttering. One notable exception is a study by Brutten and Dancer (1980) on the effect of two different types of practice on stuttering frequency. Their findings demonstrate that a greater reduction of stuttering occurred during blocked practice (each word repeated 5 times before proceeding to the next word on a list) than during random practice (the list was read in its entirety five times in succession). Unfortunately, however, this is one of the few studies on the principles of motor learning and stuttering. Needed are studies of the effects of any practice schedule on stuttering frequency during a retention task (designed to determine if a new skill was in fact learned vs. a temporary improvement in motor performance). Another important variable that needs to be addressed is the effect of speech rate during practice as well as retention tasks. Such studies are timely, particularly with children, as changes in health care bring increased demands to document effectiveness of treatment. Evidence that certain types of practice are effective with children near the onset of stuttering would have a valuable impact on clinical management of children who stutter.

Summary and Conclusions

We believe that effective treatment for children who stutter can only be provided if clinicians understand what is known as well as hypothesized regarding the programming/planning and execution of speech movements in this population. Clearly, while there is much that is unknown, there is evidence that a motor speech perspective provides a framework that will increase our understanding of this disorder. Moreover, the potential benefits of motor learning to fluency disorders in children is compelling and could be an important application in fluency treatment. We believe that increased understanding of the disorder of stuttering, sensorimotor processes underlying (dis)fluent speech and motor learning theory will help clinicians become more confident in there abilities to treat children who stutter (Sommers & Caruso, 1995). While no one approach is likely to be appropriate for all children who stutter, the strategies discussed in this paper are likely to facilitate change in articulatory kinematics (see Caruso & Strand, 1999). Of course, there is more to stuttering treatment than kinematics. The nature of the communication environment and the attitude of the child who stutters are only 2 of several other variables. However, the "bottom line" of stuttering treatment is increased fluency--which can only be achieved if the child who stutters learns to move the vocal tract structures in ways that are associated with or contribute to the production of fluent speech. In that light, a motor speech perspective on stuttering can provide a framework to enhance clinicians understanding of this disorder as well as ways to manage it.

Bibliography And Additional Selected Readings

Note: Readers of previous versions of this paper suggested that I include additional readings in stuttering, speech motor control and motor in light of the diverse audience attending this online conference. I hope you will find the below additional readings (which include the references cited in the body of this paper) to be helpful.. --AJC

Adams, M. R. (1987). Voice onsets and segment durations of normal speakers and beginning stutterers. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 12, 133-139.

Andrews, G., Craig, A., Feyer, A.-M., Hoddinott, S., Howie, P., & Neilson, M. (1983). Stuttering: A review of research findings and theories circa 1982. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 48, 226-246.

Bakker, K., & Brutten, G. J. (1989). A comparative investigation of the laryngeal premotor, adjustment, and reaction times of stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 239-244.

Bakker, K., & Brutten, G. J. (1990). Speech-related reaction times of stutterers and nonstutterers: diagnostic implications. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 55, 295-299.

Bilto, E. W. (1941). A comparative study of certain physical abilities of children with speech defects and children with normal speech. Journal of Speech Disorders, 6, 187-203.

Bloodstein, O. (1995). A handbook on stuttering (5th ed.). San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.

Borden, G. J. (1983). Initiation versus execution time during manual and oral counting by stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 389-396.

Borden, G. J., Kim, D. H., & Spiegler, K. (1987). Acoustics of stop consonant-vowel relationships during fluent and stuttered utterances. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 12, 175-184.

Carlson, J. J. (1946). Psychosomatic study of fifty stuttering children: III. Analysis of responses on the revised Stanford-Binet. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 16, 120-126.

Caruso, A. J., Abbs, J. H., & Gracco, V. L. (1988). Kinematic analysis of multiple movement coordination during speech in stutterers. Brain, 11, 439-455.

Caruso, A. J., Chodzko-Zajko, W. J., Bidinger, D. A., & Sommers, R. K. (1994). Adults who stutter: Responses to cognitive stress. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 746-754.

Caruso, A. J., Chodzko-Zajko, W. J., & McClowry, M. T. (1995). Emotional arousal and stuttering: the impact of cognitive stress. In H. F. M. Peters & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Stuttering: Proceedings of the First World Congress on Fluency Disorders (pp. 133-136). Nijmegen: University of Nijmegen Press.

Caruso, A. J., Conture, E. G., & Colton, R. H. (1988). Selected temporal parameters of coordination associated with stuttering in children. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 13, 57-82.

Caruso, A. J., Conture, E. G., Cudahy, E., & Schwartz, H. D. (1981). Acoustic analysis of young stutterers’ fluency. A paper presented to American Speech Language and Hearing Association.

Caruso, A.J. & Max, L. (1997). Applications of motor learning theory of stuttering research. In W. Hulstijn, H.F.M. Peters & P.H.H.M. Van Lieshout (Eds.), Speech Production: Motor control, brain research and fluency disorder (pp. 213-220). Amsterdam: Elsevier

Caruso, A.J. & Rolwing, A. (in progress). Children who stutter: Changes in speech associated with a communication partner's speech rate.

Caruso, A.J. & Strand, E.A. (1999). Clinical Management of Motor Speech Disorders in Children. New York: Thieme Medical Publishers

Conture, E. G. & Caruso, A.J. (1987). Assessment and diagnosis of childhood fluency disorders. In L. Rustin, D. Rowley and H. Purser (Eds.), Progress In The Treatment of Fluency Disorders, London: Taylor & Francis Ltd.

Conture, E. G., Colton, R. H., & Gleason, J. R. (1988). Selected temporal aspects of coordination during fluent speech of young stutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 640-653.

Conture, E. G., Rothenberg, M., & Molitor, R. D. (1986). Electroglottographic observations of young stutterers' fluency. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 29, 384-393.

Crary, M. A. (1995). Clinical evaluation of developmental motor speech disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 16, 110-125.

Cross, H. M. (1936). The motor capacities of stutterers. Archives of Speech, 1, 112-132.

Cullinan, W. & Springer, M. (1980). Voice initiation times in stuttering and nonstuttering childen. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 344-360.

De Nil, L. & Brutten, G. (1991). Voice onset times of stuttering and nonstuttering children: The influence of externally and linguistically imposed time pressure. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 16, 143-158.

Duffy, J. R. (1995). Motor speech disorders: Substrates, differential diagnosis, and management. St. Louis: Mosby.

Goodell, E. W., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1992). Acoustic evidence for the development of gestural coordination in the speech of 2-year-olds: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 707-727.

Guitar B, Schaefer HK, Donahue-Kilburg G, Bond L (1992). Parent verbal interactions and speech rate: a case study in stutteringJournal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 742-754

Healey, E. C., & Ramig, P. R. (1986). Acoustic measures of stutterers' and nonstutterers' fluency in two speech contexts. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 29, 325-331.

Healey, E. C., & Ramig, P. R. (1989). The relationship of stuttering severity and treatment length to temporal measures of stutterers' perceptually fluent speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 313-319.

Howell, P., Sackin, S., & Rustin, L. (1995). Comparison of speech motor development in stutterers and fluent speakers between 7 and 12 years old. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 20, 243-256.

Howell, P., Williams, M. & Young, K. (1991). Production of vowels by stuttering children and teenagers. In: H. Peters, W. Hulstijn, C. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech Motor Control and Stuttering (p. 365-384). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.

Kelly, E., Smith, A. & Goffman, L. (1995) Orofacial muscle activity of children who stutter: A preliminary study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 28, 1025-1036

Kent, R. D. (1990). Research needs in the assessment of speech motor disorders. In J. A. Cooper (Ed.), Assessment of speech and voice production: Research and clinical applications (pp. 17-29). Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health.

Klich, R. S., & May, G. M. (1982). Spectrographic study of vowels in stutterers' fluent speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 25, 364-370.

Kopp, H. (1946). Psychosomatic study of fifty stuttering children: II. Ozeretzky tests. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 16, 114-119.

Logan, KJ & Caruso, AJ (1997) Parents as partners in the treatment of childhood stuttering. Seminars in Speech Language, 18(4):309-26

Max, L., Caruso, A.J. & Vandevenne, A. (1997). Decreased stuttering frequency during repeated readings: a motor learning perspective. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 22, 1-17.

McKnight, R. & Cullinan, W. (1987). Subgroups of stuttering children: Speech and voice reaction times, segmental durations and naming latencies. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 12, 317-324.

Molt, L. F. (1991). Selected acoustic and physiologic measures of speech motor coordination in stuttering and nonstuttering children. In H. F. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering (433-440). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Neilson, M. D., & Neilson, P. D. (1991). Adaptive model theory of speech motor control and stuttering. In H. F. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering (149-156). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Peters, H. F. M., & Hulstijn, W. (1987b). Programming and initiation of speech utterances in stuttering. In H. F. M. Peters & W. Hulstijn (Eds.), Speech motor dynamics in stuttering (185-195). Wien, Austria: Springer-Verlag.

Peters, H. F. M., Hulstijn, W., & Starkweather, C. W (1989). Acoustic and physiologic reaction times of stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 668-680.

Pindzola, R. H. (1987). Durational characteristics of the fluent speech of stutterers and nonstutterers. Folia Phoniatrica, 39, 90-97.

Riley, G. D., & Riley, J. (1979). A component model for diagnosing and treating children who stutter. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 4, 279-293.

Riley, G., & Riley, J. (1991). Treatment implications of oral motor discoordination. In H. F.M. Peters, W. Hulstijn, & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering (pp. 471-476). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Robb, M. P., Lybolt, J. T., & Price, H. A. (1985). Acoustic measures of stutterers' speech following an intensive therapy program. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 10, 269-279.

Ryan, B. (1974). Programmed therapy for stuttering in children and adults. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas

Schmidt, R..A. (1988). Motor Control and learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Schmidt, R.A. (1991). Motor learning & performance. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Sim, H. & Zebrowski, P. (1994). Acoustic analysis of the pre-and post-treatment fluent speech of children who stutter: Preliminary observations. NCVS Status and Progress Report, 6, 81-88.

Snyder, M. A. (1958). Stuttering and coordination: An investigation of the relationship between the stutterer's coordination and his speech difficulty. Logos, 1, 36-44.

Sommers, R.J. & Caruso, A.J. (1995). Inservice training in speech pathology: are we meeting the needs for fluency training? American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 4(3), 22-28.

Story, R., & Alfonso, P. (1989). Temporal recognition of lip/jaw gestures following an intensive therapy program [Abstract]. Asha, 31, 65.

Strand, E. (1995). Treatment of motor speech disorders in children. Seminars in Speech and Language, 16, 126-139.

Strother, C. R., & Kriegman, L. S. (1943). Diadochokinesis in stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech Disorders, 8, 323-335.

Thelen, E. (1991). Motor aspects of emergent speech: A dynamic approach. In N. Krasnegor (Ed.), Biobehavioral Foundations of Language (pp. 339-362). New York: Erlbaum.

Van Riper, C. (1982). The nature of stuttering. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

Vaughn, C.-L. D., & Webster, W. G. (1989). Bimanual handedness in adults who stutterers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 375-382.

Williams, H. G., & Bishop, J. H. (1992). Speed and consistency of manual movements of stutterers, articulation-disordered children, and children with normal speech. Journal Fluency Disorders, 17, 191-203.

Yairi, E. (1990). Subtyping child stutterers for research purposes. In J. A. Cooper (Ed.), Research needs in stuttering: Roadblocks and future direction (pp. 50-57). Rockville, MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

Yaruss, J. & Conture, E. (1996). Stuttering and phonological disorders in children: Examination of the covert repair hypothesis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1-16.

Zebrowski, P. M. (1991). Preliminary acoustic analysis of the perceptually fluent speech of stuttering children before and after intensive therapy. In H. F. M. Peters, W. Hulstijn & C. W. Starkweather (Eds.), Speech motor control and stuttering (423-432). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

Zebrowski, P. M., Conture, E. G., & Cudahy, E. A. (1985). Acoustic analysis of young stutterers' fluency: preliminary observations. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 10, 173-192.

Zebrowski, P. M., & Moon, J. B. (1996, June). Visuomotor tracking ability of children who stutter: A preliminary study. Poster session presented at the Third International Conference on Speech Motor Production and Fluency Disorders, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

Zimmermann, G. (1980a). Articulatory dynamics of fluent utterances of stutterers and nonstutterers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 95-107.

Zimmermann, G. (1980b). Stuttering: A disorder of movement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 23, 122-136.


You can post Questions/comments about the above paper to Anthony Caruso before October 22, 2000.


September 21, 2000